The catalogue contains study descriptions in various languages. The system searches with your search terms from study descriptions available in the language you have selected. The catalogue does not have ‘All languages’ option as due to linguistic differences this would give incomplete results. See the User Guide for more detailed information.
Trust in advisors: Origins, effects, and implications for risk communication
Creator
Harvey, N, University College London
Study number / PID
850006 (UKDA)
10.5255/UKDA-SN-850006 (DOI)
Data access
Restricted
Series
Not available
Abstract
It is now well recognised that trust in sources of advice is important for effective risk communication. To maintain some influence over peoples opinions and risk-taking behaviours, government departments, medical bodies and other agencies must endeavour to maintain the trust that people have in them. When trust is lost, so is influence. As an example of this, commentators have pointed to effects of poor advice during the bovine spongiform encephalopathy (mad cow) crisis. The government assured people that it was safe to eat beef but later this behaviour was later linked to a brain disease that is always fatal. This reduced trust in the government as a source of advice about risk for many people. As a result, there appears to be a reluctance to take government advice on other matters. For example, the number of measles cases has increased in many parts of the country: fewer people are having their children vaccinated against the disease because they do not believe the governments assurances that the MMR vaccine is safe. Recently, Onora ONeill, in her 2002 Reith lectures, has argued that we should be cautious about accepting peoples claims that they have lost trust in some agency as evidence that they have actually lost their trust in it. For example, she suggests that many people who say they no longer trust supermarket food still shop in supermarkets rather than elsewhere. For her, it is important to make a distinction between stated trust and actual trust. Our aim is to find out more about trust by answering a number of questions about it while keeping ONeills distincion in mind. What determines trust? How should it be measured? Does it have similar effects on peoples estimates about levels of risk and their risk-related behaviours? How valid is the view that it is easier to destroy than to create trust? Is trust in advisors modified in a rational way? If not, what determines how it is modified? Does trust (or lack of it) transfer across domains? Answers...
Terminology used is generally based on DDI controlled vocabularies: Time Method, Analysis Unit, Sampling Procedure and Mode of Collection, available at CESSDA Vocabulary Service.
Methodology
Data collection period
01/10/2003 - 30/09/2006
Country
United Kingdom
Time dimension
Not available
Analysis unit
Individual
Universe
Not available
Sampling procedure
Not available
Kind of data
Numeric
Data collection mode
Experimental. All 22 studies were empirical experiments run on students at University College London. Participants saw stimuli on a computer screen and entered their responses.Data set includes 22 experiments, with one folder per experiment, each containing a brief summary of the experiment, a sheet of raw data, and a copy of all experimental instructions – all in .txt format. Also included are a general summary of all 22 experiments, a key explaining column labels in the data summary files, and an additional ‘infosheet’ in the Experiment 10 folder describing the questionnaire used in that study.
Funding information
Grant number
RES-000-23-0114
Access
Publisher
UK Data Service
Publication year
2008
Terms of data access
The Data Collection is available for download to users registered with the UK Data Service.