Summary information

Study title

Turning novel words into known words: why ostensive naming facilitates children's word learning via fast mapping

Creator

Horst, J, University of Sussex

Study number / PID

850709 (UKDA)

10.5255/UKDA-SN-850709 (DOI)

Data access

Restricted

Series

Not available

Abstract

The ability understand others and to be understood is a complex challenge for children. This research examines the mechanisms that support successful word learning in young children. Children are very good at forming a rough idea of what a new word means. For example, if shown a book and a novel electronic gadget and asked for the 'iPod' young children use process-of-elimination to link 'iPod' to the gadget. However, this is just the beginning. To really learn words children must form strong memories of the words and their meanings. Recently, we demonstrated that children only learn from process-of-elimination if ostensive naming is used. Ostensive naming involves naming something by making it very obvious what the word refers to, usually by holding up and pointing to the target object. This research builds on this finding and explores which aspects of ostensive naming make it so helpful. Experiment 1 explores highlighting the target object and decreasing children's attention to all other objects. Experiment 2 explores the distances between the objects when they are being named. Overall this research will increase our understanding of mechanisms that support early word learning, when children are already speaking but still have a lot to learn.

Keywords

Methodology

Data collection period

02/05/2011 - 01/09/2012

Country

United Kingdom

Time dimension

Not available

Analysis unit

Household
Individual

Universe

Not available

Sampling procedure

Not available

Kind of data

Numeric

Data collection mode

General Methods: Children were tested individually at the lab with their parents seated next to them. As in previous studies (e.g. Horst & Samuelson, 2008) children received warm-up trials to become familiar with the task. Then, children were presented with referent selection (AKA fast mapping) trials. On referent selection trials children saw two known objects (e.g. stuffed dog, toy car) and one novel object (e.g. birdtoy). Across trials children were asked for both novel and known names to ensure they were responding to the requested names and not simply to novelty. To correctly choose the novel object in response to a novel name, children could use process-of-elimination (‘fode’ cannot refer to the dog or the car because those are called something else).Finally, all children were presented with retention trials to test if they learned the novel names. On these trials children saw only novel objects, each of which had been a target during the referent selection trials. To correctly choose the novel targets on these trials, children needed to have learned the name-object associations (and remembered them after a short delay).Children’s proportion of correct choices were compared against chance (1/3) and between groups. At least 12 children participated in each condition, as in previous studies (e.g. Horst, Scott & Pollard, 2010).

Funding information

Grant number

RES-000-22-4451

Access

Publisher

UK Data Service

Publication year

2013

Terms of data access

The Data Collection is available for download to users registered with the UK Data Service.

Related publications

Not available