The catalogue contains study descriptions in various languages. The system searches with your search terms from study descriptions available in the language you have selected. The catalogue does not have ‘All languages’ option as due to linguistic differences this would give incomplete results. See the User Guide for more detailed information.
Access to Justice for Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2013-2016
Creator
Henry, L, City, University of London
Wilcock, R, University of Winchester
Crane, L, City, University of London
Study number / PID
855245 (UKDA)
10.5255/UKDA-SN-852471 (DOI)
Data access
Restricted
Series
Not available
Abstract
Data collection included two distinct groups of participants.
(1) Data collected from 202 typically developing children and 72 autistic children, all in the age range 6-11 years when first seen. (Note that one child on the autism spectrum and one child with typical development had IQ scores below 70.) Data collected from the children included standardised and non-standardised tests for a range of cognitive skills (IQ, language, memory, attention, suggestibility, anxiety) as well as data from the four experimental phases of the research: brief initial interviews; full investigative interviews; identification line-ups; and cross-examinations (note that not all children participated in this final phase).
(2) Data collected from three samples of jury eligible adults (ages 18-69, n=260 in total) concerning their credibility ratings of interviews (and some cross-examinations) of selected children with and without autism.Children with a diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) display some memory and social communication difficulties that may call into question their abilities as witnesses. The current research examined how children with and without a diagnosis of ASD fared during all the different stages of a criminal investigation, and considered the best ways of eliciting reliable evidence. There were four research questions:
1. How do children with a diagnosis of ASD fare during the different stages of a criminal investigation (initial questioning, an investigative interview, when identifying perpetrators, and during cross-examination) relative to typical children?
2. What can be done to improve the amount of information that children recall, without making them more prone to errors; for example, is the assistance of a Registered Intermediary - a professional communications specialist who assists vulnerable individuals within the criminal justice system - useful?
3. How do the general public – who may be evaluating evidence within a jury -...
Terminology used is generally based on DDI controlled vocabularies: Time Method, Analysis Unit, Sampling Procedure and Mode of Collection, available at CESSDA Vocabulary Service.
Methodology
Data collection period
01/04/2013 - 12/07/2016
Country
United Kingdom
Time dimension
Not available
Analysis unit
Individual
Universe
Not available
Sampling procedure
Not available
Kind of data
Numeric
Data collection mode
(1). Data from child 'witnesses'. Samples of typically developing children (6- to 11-year-olds) were recruited from five mainstream schools in Greater London. Samples of children with an autism diagnosis (6- to 11-year-olds) were recruited from mainstream schools in Greater London, special schools in Greater London, and via parent support networks in Essex and Kent. Children were assessed with standardised cognitive tests (and some unstandardized tasks). Procedure for the mock criminal investigation. 1. Children viewed an event at school (either live or via a video) about school in Victorian times involving two male adult actors, one of whom committed a minor theft. 2. Children received a brief interview about this event on the same day - akin to a first response police officer taking an initial statement. 3. One week later children received a full investigative interview and viewed two identification line-ups (relating to the two male actors). Prior to this, children were semi-randomly allocated into four interview conditions: a best-practice police interview; a Verbal Labels interview (additional support from distinct questions about people, actions/objects, setting and conversations); a Sketch Reinstatement of Context interview (using a drawing technique to re-orientate the child to the context of the event); or a Registered Intermediary assisted interview (a fully-qualified experienced intermediary facilitated the child's communication throughout the interview and identification line-up). 4. 8-13 months later (average 11 months), available children in the sample were cross-examined by experienced barristers over Skype to mimic the 'live link' used in court. The majority of these children were in the typically developing group. A defence statement was prepared and the barrister challenged the child on seven 'false' points that were taken from this statement, attempting to get them to agree to erroneous information. Scoring for the child witnesses data. i) All standardised tests were scored according to the relevant manuals. ii) Interviews (Brief and Investigative) were recorded, transcribed and scored according to clear and robustly derived criteria based on the number of 'units of correct information' recalled. These data were additionally scored for the types of details recalled (e.g. numbers of person, action, object, setting, conversation, and general details). Numbers of incorrect details (information that was inaccurate) and confabulations (information that did not occur in the event) were also recorded. iii) Line-up choices were scored as correct identifications, incorrect identifications, incorrect rejections, or correct rejections. iv) Mock cross-examinations were scored based on how many times, from a maximum of seven false points in the 'defence statement', the child complied with the false suggestions posed by the barrister. Higher scores were given for those who were more resistant to the barristers' challenges of this false information. We also recorded the extent to which children had provided information about these seven points in their investigative interviews (this was the 'memory trace score' - higher scores were given to those who provided fuller details). Finally, we coded the types of questions barristers asked and the types of responses given by the children (these data are only available for participants with typical development). (2). Data from 'mock jurors'. Jury eligible adults (18-69 years) were recruited via convenience sampling from universities, workplaces and personal contacts. These participants rated selected interviews/cross-examinations from our child witnesses dataset on a series of 7-point scales, reflecting how they appraised the credibility of that individual witness. Study 1 (n=120) - ratings were made of interviews with children who had an autism diagnosis. Two autistic children provided these interviews and half of the adult participants rated each video. Further, the adult participants were divided into three groups, randomly assigned to receive: (1) no information about the child's autism diagnosis; (2) information that the child had a diagnosis of autism; or (3) information that the child had a diagnosis of autism together with a summary of the key characteristics of autism. Study 2 (n=60) – ratings were made of two videos of autistic children; one child received a best-practice police interview and the other child was assisted by a Registered Intermediary. Participants rated one video and were randomly allocated to one of the two video conditions. Study 3 (n=80) - ratings were made of interviews and cross-examinations of four typically developing children: two of these children were assisted by a Registered Intermediary and two of these children received a best-practice (standard) police interview. In each pair of children, one child was 'higher' ability and one child was 'lower' ability and these pairs were matched across interview conditions. Participants each rated one interview and were randomly allocated to one of the four conditions.
Funding information
Grant number
ES/J020893/2
Access
Publisher
UK Data Service
Publication year
2021
Terms of data access
The Data Collection is available for download to users registered with the UK Data Service. All requests are subject to the permission of the data owner or his/her nominee. Please email the contact person for this data collections to request permission to access the data, explaining your reason for wanting access to the data. Once permission is obtained, please forward this to the ReShare administrator.