The catalogue contains study descriptions in various languages. The system searches with your search terms from study descriptions available in the language you have selected. The catalogue does not have ‘All languages’ option as due to linguistic differences this would give incomplete results. See the User Guide for more detailed information.
International Centre for Language and Communicative Development: How the Input Shapes the Acquisition of Verb Morphology: Elicited Production and Computational Modelling in Two Highly Inflected Languages, 2014-2020
Creator
Engelmann, F, University of Manchester
Granlund, S, University of Liverpool
Kolak, J, University of Salford
Szreder, M, University of Manchester
Ambridge, B, University of Liverpool
Pine, J, University of Liverpool
Theakston, A, University of Manchester
Lieven, E, University of Manchester
Study number / PID
853914 (UKDA)
10.5255/UKDA-SN-853914 (DOI)
Data access
Open
Series
Not available
Abstract
The aim of the present work was to develop a computational model of how children acquire inflectional morphology for marking person and number; one of the central challenges in language development. First, in order to establish which putative learning phenomena are sufficiently robust to constitute a target for modelling, we ran large-scale elicited production studies with native learners of Finnish (N = 77; 35–63 months) and Polish (N = 81; 35–59 months), using a novel method that, unlike previous studies, allows for elicitation of all six person/number forms in the paradigm (first, second and third person; singular and plural). We then proceeded to build and test a connectionist model of the acquisition of person/number marking which not only acquires near adult-like mastery of the system (including generalisation to unseen items), but also yields all of the key phenomena observed in the elicited-production studies; specifically, effects of token frequency and phonological neighbourhood density of the target form, and a pattern whereby errors generally reflect the replacement of low frequency targets by higher-frequency forms of the same verb, or forms with the same person/number as the target, but with a suffix from an inappropriate conjugation class. The findings demonstrate that acquisition of even highly complex systems of inflectional morphology can be accounted for by a theoretical model that assumes rote storage and phonological analogy, as opposed to formal symbolic rules.The International Centre for Language and Communicative Development (LuCiD) will bring about a transformation in our understanding of how children learn to communicate, and deliver the crucial information needed to design effective interventions in child healthcare, communicative development and early years education.
Learning to use language to communicate is hugely important for society. Failure to develop language and communication skills at the right age is a major predictor of...
Terminology used is generally based on DDI controlled vocabularies: Time Method, Analysis Unit, Sampling Procedure and Mode of Collection, available at CESSDA Vocabulary Service.
Methodology
Data collection period
01/09/2014 - 31/05/2020
Country
Finland, Poland
Time dimension
Not available
Analysis unit
Individual
Universe
Not available
Sampling procedure
Not available
Kind of data
Numeric
Text
Audio
Data collection mode
80 children were tested in Finnish, and 91 were tested in Polish. A total of thirteen children were excluded, resulting in a final sample of 77 Finnish children, and 81 Polish. All children were reported to be typically-developing monolingual speakers. The study used a within-subjects design with an elicited production paradigm; the stimuli consisted of 32 verbs in each language with accompanying videos presented on a laptop computer. All verbs were chosen to be familiar to young children, and suitable for illustration in cartoon animations. For each language, we created 10 pseudo-randomized lists, each containing 16 of the 32 verbs. Each child completed one list (i.e., half of the total experimental design) in order to minimize fatigue. For each of the 16 verbs assigned to each participant, we attempted to elicit all six present tense forms (1sg, 2sg, 3sg, 1pl, 2pl, 3pl) in Polish, and all but 3pl (which is not commonly used in spoken language) in Finnish, for a total of 96 and 80 trials per participant in Polish and Finnish, respectively. Each child was tested individually in a quiet setting, and completed three sessions: a training/practice session (20–25 min) and two experimental sessions (each 15–25 min), each containing half of the test stimuli for each child (i.e., 48 trials in Polish and 40 in Finnish). The child was seated in front of the laptop computer with a ‘talking’ toy fox positioned next to the laptop, facing the child and the experimenter. The toy fox’s internal speakers were connected to the laptop. The child was told that she would be playing a game with the experimenter in which they would watch and describe some videos of different actions. Each animation ended on an informative freeze-frame which clearly depicted the action being performed. The child then produced their response, and it was audio-recorded.
Funding information
Grant number
ES/L008955/1
Access
Publisher
UK Data Service
Publication year
2021
Terms of data access
The Data Collection is available to any user without the requirement for registration for download/access.